- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Participant attention on the intervention target during repetitive passive movement improved spinal reciprocal inhibition enhancement and joint movement function
European Journal of Medical Research volume 28, Article number: 428 (2023)
Abstract
This study aimed to evaluate the effects of the participant’s attention target during repetitive passive movement (RPM) intervention on reciprocal inhibition (RI) and joint movement function. Twenty healthy adults participated in two experiments involving four attention conditions [control (forward attention with no RPM), forward attention (during RPM), monitor attention (monitor counting task during RPM), ankle joint attention (ankle movement counting task during RPM)] during 10-min RPM interventions on the ankle joint. Counting tasks were included to ensure the participant’s attention remained on the target during the intervention. In Experiment 1, RI was measured before, immediately after, and 5, 10, 15, 20, and 30 min after the RPM intervention. In Experiment 2, we evaluated ankle joint movement function at the same time points before and after RPM intervention. The maximum ankle dorsiflexion movement (from 30° plantar flexion to 10° dorsiflexion) was measured, reflecting RI. In Experiment 1, the RI function reciprocal Ia inhibition was enhanced for 10 min after RPM under all attention conditions (excluding the control condition. D1 inhibition was enhanced for 20 min after RPM in the forward and monitor attention conditions and 30 min after RPM in the ankle joint attention condition. In Experiment 2, the joint movement function decreased under the forward and monitor attention conditions but improved under the ankle joint attention condition. This study is the first to demonstrate that the participant’s attention target affected the intervention effect of the RI enhancement method, which has implications for improving the intervention effect of rehabilitation.
Background
Decreases in spinal reciprocal inhibition (RI) function are observed in patients with upper motor neuron disorders [1,2,3] and older adults [4,5,6,7,8,9]. Decreases in RI function cause excessive simultaneous activity between antagonistic muscles, impair smooth joint movement and walking function, and increase the risk of falls [10, 11].
In recent years, several studies have used techniques to enhance RI to inhibit excessive simultaneous muscle activity [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. Approaches for enhancing RI include patterned electrical stimulation (PES) [15, 17, 21, 23, 24] and repetitive passive movement (RPM) [12, 13] peripheral stimulation, which show longer intervention after-effects than brain stimulation [14, 16, 18, 21]. In addition, in peripheral stimulation, compared to the after-effects at 10Â min after a 20Â min PES intervention, the effects of a 10Â min RPM intervention persisted until 20Â min after the intervention, indicating improved after-effects with a shorter intervention time [12, 13]. Furthermore, the combined use of brain stimulation [transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), intermittent theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation (iTBS)] and peripheral stimulation synergistically improved the effects on RI after the intervention [21, 23]. RI was also enhanced by the combined use of motor imagery and peripheral electrical stimulation [25], indicating that primary motor cortex activity and increased excitability of the corticospinal tract enhanced the effects of peripheral stimulation.
Our research group focuses on simpler attention techniques than motor imagery training. The active attention process has been shown to play an important role in motor learning [26, 27] and motor performance [28, 29]. It has been reported that directing attention to behavior during a simple motor task activates the prefrontal area, cingulate cortex, supplementary motor area, premotor area, and cerebellum [30, 31] and contributes to the connection between the prefrontal and premotor areas [31]. It is recognized that motor-related areas are activated when attention is directed to the intervention target. [32] reported that attention to the index finger during movement attenuated short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) and increased corticospinal tract excitability.
Like brain stimulation and motor imagery, attention to the intervention target during RI enhancement may increase the intervention effect of peripheral stimuli. From previous studies [12, 13], we can expect to achieve enhanced intervention effects by directing attention to the intervention target during RPM, which as a method of RI augmentation can produce a large intervention effect in a short time. Examining the combined effects of RPM and attention to the intervention target will facilitate the development of more effective rehabilitation methods for clinical application. In addition, among the few reports on the post-intervention effect of the RPM RI enhancement method, only walking, balance function, and dynamic performance have been considered, and it is necessary to examine the intervention effect on a basic single joint movement. Because RI is the most important function during joint movement, we examined the intervention effect of the RI enhancement method on joint movement function.
Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the effect of RI enhancement by focusing attention on the target during the RPM intervention and evaluate the intervention effect via joint movement function. The hypothesis of this study is that attention to the intervention target during RPM intervention will inhibit SICI, increase corticospinal tract excitability, activate RI-inhibitory interneurons, and enhance RI potentiation. Moreover, we hypothesize that the joint movement function improvements will be modulated by RI enhancement and corticospinal tract excitability.
Methods
We conducted two experiments to investigate how focusing attention on the target during RPM intervention affects RI and joint movement function. In Experiment 1, we set attention targets under four conditions during the RPM RI enhancement intervention and assessed RI before and after the intervention. In Experiment 2, the same four conditions were set, and the joint movement function was examined before and after the RPM intervention to examine the effect of the intervention.
Study participants
Twenty healthy adults (age, 20.8 ± 0.9 years; height, 166.7 ± 7.8 cm; body weight, 57.5 ± 8.2 kg; 10 females) provided written informed consent to participate in this study. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the Niigata University of Health and Welfare (18,267–190,918). All experiments were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Niigata University of Health and Welfare and the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments.
Experimental protocol overview
The experimental protocol is illustrated in Fig. 1. In Experiment 1, the stimulus intensities of the conditioned stimulus and the test stimulus were set before the RI measurement. RI was measured before (Pre), immediately after (Post), 5 min after (Post 5), 10 min after (Post 10), 20 min after (Post 20), and 30 min after (Post 30) the RPM intervention. In both experiments, we applied four attention conditions: control, forward attention, monitor attention, and ankle joint attention. In Experiment 2, the joint movement function was performed before and after the RPM intervention to examine the intervention effect on RI. Joint movement function was measured 5 min after the end of the intervention when RI enhancement is most apparent [12, 13]. A 5 min break was given between each session.
Electromyography (EMG)
The distance between the Ag/AgCl electrodes (Blue Sensor, METS, Tokyo, Japan) for the surface electromyogram was 20 mm. The electrodes were placed on the tibialis anterior (TA) and soleus (Sol) muscles according to the surface EMG for non-invasive assessment of muscles protocols [33]. A ground electrode was placed between the electrical stimulation electrode and the TA surface electromyogram electrode [34, 35]. Electromyographic activity was filtered at a bandpass filter of 10–1000 Hz and amplified 100 × (FA-DL-720–140; 4Assist, Tokyo, Japan) before being digitally stored (10 kHz sampling rate) on a personal computer for offline analysis. Data analysis was performed using PowerLab 8/30 and LabChart 7 (both AD Instruments, Colorado Springs, CO, USA).
RPM intervention
We adopted the RPM parameters that most effectively enhanced RI in our previous studies [12, 13]; specifically, an intervention time of 10 min, a movement speed of 160°/s, and a movement range of 30° ankle plantar flexion to 10° dorsiflexion. The participant’s thighs and feet were fixed to the seat surface and the foot plate, respectively, to maintain the lower limb position throughout the experiment (Takei Scientific Instruments, Niigata, Japan).
Attention conditions
Four attention conditions (control, forward attention, monitor attention, and ankle joint attention) were randomly applied during a 10 min RPM intervention, with an interval of at least 3 days between each condition. For the control condition, the participant was sat in a chair for 10 min with attention to the front wall (without RPM intervention). For the forward attention condition, the participant’s attention was directed to the anterior wall during the 10-min RPM intervention. For the monitor attention condition, the participant’s attention was directed to the monitor in front of them during the 10-min RPM intervention, and they were asked to count the circles displayed on the monitor at 1 s intervals. The circle was randomly displayed 25–35 times, hidden for 10 s, and then displayed again (Fig. 2). During the period when the circle was not displayed, the participant was asked to report to the examiner the number of times the circle was displayed while also maintaining attention to the monitor. For the ankle joint attention condition, the participant was asked to count the number of ankle joint movements during the 10-min RPM intervention. The count was taken at the time of maximum ankle joint dorsiflexion. The examiner instructed the participant when to start and pause the count. The counting time was a cycle in which the range of 25–35 s was randomly counted. The counting was then paused for 10 s and resumed at the signal of the examiner. The participant reported the number of counts to the examiner during the count pause, and the participant was instructed to maintain attention to the ankle joint during the 10-s count pause. The participants were asked to perform the count to ensure that the ankle joint was being observed [32]. The effect of the counting task was examined by comparing the monitor counting attention condition with the forward attention condition.
RI measurement
The RI measurement method is described in detail in our previous publications [12,13,14]. Briefly, an electrical stimulation device (SEN-8203, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan) was used to apply electrical stimulation (1 ms duration, square wave) via an isolator (SS-104J, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). We measured the RI of the pathway that inhibits Sol (test stimulus) from TA (conditional stimulus). The conditioned stimulus stimulated the common fibular nerve, which is the dominant nerve of TA, and the stimulus intensity was set to the M wave threshold (≤ 100 µV). The test stimulus stimulated the tibial nerve, which is the dominant nerve of Sol, and the stimulus intensity was set to achieve a Sol H-reflex amplitude value of 15–25% of the maximum M wave amplitude value (Mmax). The RI stimulation conditions were a 2 ms conditioning stimulation-test stimulation interval (CT-interval 2 ms), a 20 ms conditioning stimulation-test stimulation interval (CT-interval 20 ms), and a test stimulus without a conditioned stimulus (single). The stimulation frequency was 0.3 Hz. The CT-interval 2 ms generates the largest amount of reciprocal Ia inhibition [36, 37], and CT-interval 20 ms generates the largest amount of D1 inhibition [36].
Joint movement function
The task movement of the joint movement function was the ankle dorsiflexion movement. After 5 min of pre-attention, the ankle dorsiflexion task was performed three times before and after each attention condition with a rest interval of 10 s between tasks. Like Experiment 1, the task movement range was from 30° ankle plantar flexion to 10° dorsiflexion. The participant was seated with their arms folded in front of their chest and instructed to quickly start the dorsiflexion with maximum effort in their own timing after the examiner’s signal.
Data analysis
Experiment 1
For the RI data analysis, the peak-to-peak values of the waveform amplitudes (12 waveforms) under each stimulus condition were averaged, and the Sol H reflection amplitude value and the M wave amplitude value were calculated. The RI was calculated by dividing the Sol H reflection amplitude value by the maximum M wave amplitude value in percent notation (Sol H-reflex amplitude as the percentage of Mmax). When comparing the changes over time in each attention condition, the H-reflex amplitude value of the test stimulus with the conditioned stimulus was divided by the H-reflex amplitude value of the test stimulus and presented as a percentage ([Amplitude of conditioned H-reflex amplitude/test H-reflex amplitude] × 100).
Experiment 2
The analysis items for the ankle dorsiflexion task were TA and Sol EMG, co-contraction index (CI), ankle dorsiflexion peak torque (PT), and rate of joint movement development (RJD). The analysis sections are (1), from the start of TA EMG to the start of the joint movement; (2), from the start of the joint movement to the end of the joint movement; and (3), from the start of TA EMG to the end of the joint movement. The starting point of TA EMG was defined as the time point when the resting mean EMG ± 3 standard deviations (SD) was exceeded. The EMG of TA and Sol was calculated as the average EMG of each analysis range divided by the maximum voluntary contraction (MVC). The ankle dorsiflexion PT analyzed the maximum value of the ankle dorsiflexion torque of the task movement. The RJD was calculated by dividing the joint angle (40°) from the start to the end of the movement task by the time (s). The CI calculation method is as follows [38]:
where Iant is the area of the waveform on which the antagonist muscles acted, calculated using the following equation:
where t1 to t2 indicate the period in which Sol EMG is less active than TA EMG, and t2 to t3 indicate the period in which TA EMG is less active than Sol EMG. Itotal is the total integral value of Sol and TA EMG calculated using the following formula:
Statistical processing
For Experiment 1, repeated measures three-way ANOVA was performed to compare the attention condition (control, forward attention, monitor attention, ankle joint attention) × the stimulus condition (single, CT-interval 2 ms, CT-interval 20 ms) × the measurement time (Pre, Post, Post 5, Post 10, Post 20, Post 30). As a post-test, the comparison of the stimulus conditions of each attention condition was performed by applying the Bonferroni correction to the paired t test. To compare the measurement time of each attention condition, the Bonferroni correction was applied to the paired t test. For Experiment 2, paired t tests were used to compare the pre and post results in each analysis interval. The significance level was set to 5% for all analyses.
Results
Experiment 1
Sol background EMG, Sol Mmax amplitude values, and TA M wave amplitude values are shown in Tables 1, 2, and 3. Representative Sol and TA waveforms are shown in Fig. 3. The results of repeated measures three-way ANOVA did not detect a significant effect of the attention condition [F(3, 57) = 2.575, p = 0.063, partial η2 = 0.119], but detected significant effects of the stimulus condition [F(2, 38) = 176.473, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.903], the measurement time [F(5, 95) = 8.822, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.317], and the interaction of three factors [F(30, 570) = 4.426, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.189].
There was no significant difference in the H-reflex amplitude value under the single condition at each measurement time under each attention condition (Table 4). Therefore, the change in the Sol H-reflex amplitude value with respect to the conditioned stimuli was not dependent on the test stimulus intensity.
The single condition obtained at each measurement time of each attention condition was compared with the CT-interval conditions. The H-reflex amplitude value was significantly lower under the CT-interval 2 ms and CT-interval 20 ms conditions than under the single condition at all measurement times under all attention conditions (p < 0.001, Table 4). The results demonstrated that reciprocal Ia inhibition (CT-interval 2 ms) and D1 inhibition (CT-interval 20 ms) occurred under all conditions.
The H-reflex amplitude value at the measurement times under CT-interval 2 ms and CT-interval 20 ms were compared with the Pre measurement (Fig. 4A, B). Under CT-interval 2 ms, the control condition did not show a significant difference in the H-reflex amplitude value compared with Pre. In the forward attention condition, monitor attention condition, and ankle joint attention condition, the H-reflex amplitude value was significantly reduced at Post 5 and Post 10 compared with Pre (p < 0.001). Similarly, the H-reflex amplitude values under the control condition did not significantly differ from Pre under CT-interval 20 ms. In the forward attention condition and the monitor attention condition, the H-reflex amplitude value was significantly lower at Post 5, Post 10, and Post 20 compared with Pre (p < 0.001). As for the ankle joint attention condition, the H-reflex amplitude value was significantly reduced at Post 5, Post 10, Post 20, and Post 30 compared with Pre (p < 0.001).
Experiment 2
Comparing the EMG Pre and Post results within each attention condition revealed a significant increase in EMG for TA (p < 0.01) and a significant decrease in EMG for Sol (p < 0.05) for all analysis sessions in the ankle joint attention condition (Fig. 5A–D).
Comparing the Pre and Post ankle dorsiflexion PT within each attention condition, the ankle dorsiflexion PT was significantly reduced under the forward attention condition and the monitor attention condition (p < 0.001). Under the ankle joint attention condition, the ankle dorsiflexion PT was significantly increased (p < 0.001).
Comparing the Pre and Post RJD within each attention condition, the RJD significantly decreased under the forward attention condition and the monitor attention condition (p < 0.001). The RJD significantly increased under the ankle joint attention condition (p < 0.001).
Comparing the pre- and post-CI within each attention condition, the CI decreased significantly in the analysis sessions of ankle joint attention condition (2) (p < 0.01) and (3) (p < 0.001).
Discussion
The results of this study demonstrated that the duration of RI enhancement could be extended by focusing the participant’s attention on the ankle joint targeted during the RPM RI enhancement intervention. The joint movement function evaluation revealed that the joint movement function decreased with RPM intervention alone and improved when the participant focused their attention on the ankle joint during the RPM intervention.
In this study, we instructed the participant to perform a counting task to confirm that the participant’s attention was focused on the target. The results of Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 showed that the effects during the counting of ankle joint movements (ankle joint attention condition) were similar to those of the forward attention condition and the monitor attention condition, which involved counting the appearances of a circle on the monitor, indicating the effect of counting was negligible. This finding is consistent with the results of a previous report [32], who showed that the effect of counting on an attention task during index finger RPM was extremely small.
In this study, the RI enhancement effect of RPM intervention supported the results of our previous study [12, 13], i.e., reciprocal Ia inhibition is enhanced for 10 min after the intervention, and D1 inhibition is enhanced for 20 min after the intervention, demonstrating that RPM is a useful intervention method for RI enhancement. The present study revealed that the participant’s attention to the RPM intervention target (the ankle joint) extended the RI enhancement for up to 30 min after the RPM intervention, supporting our hypothesis. The effect of RPM alone on RI is attributed to the repeated muscle lengthening and shortening, which increase the firing frequency of afferent Ia fibers from muscle spindles in the TA and activate RI-inhibitory interneurons, contributing to the enhancement of RI. Similar results have been obtained using PES [15, 17, 21, 23, 24], a technique that specifically stimulates Ia fibers to enhance RI.
In addition, the activity of motor-associated cortical areas activates RI-inhibitory interneurons and contributes to RI enhancement, and the intervention effect of peripheral stimulation is enhanced by brain stimulation [14, 16,17,18, 21, 23]. The combination of brain stimulation and peripheral stimulation is effective for RI enhancement in RI-inhibitory interneurons via the convergent input from the motor cortex and the afferent fibers of TA [39,40,41,42]. In this study, as an alternative to brain stimulation, we focused the participant’s attention on the intervention target during RPM to easily increase motor cortex excitability. A previous study by our research group [32] showed that attention to the intervention target during RPM increased the corticospinal tract excitability of the target muscle. The increase in corticospinal tract excitability caused by attention to the target has been attributed to the inhibition of SICI and short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI) [43, 44]. The results of the present study suggest that the factors contributing to RI enhancement when attention was focused on the RPM intervention target may include motor cortex activation, an increase in the descending input, and RI-inhibitory interneuron activation, which prolonged the post-intervention RI enhancement effect. Previous studies have shown that motor imagery is an effective intervention method for activating the motor cortex. The RI enhancement effect achieved using motor imagery during PES intervention was comparable to that achieved using peripheral stimuli with attention directed to the target [25]. Based on the findings of these previous studies, attention to the intervention target during RPM enhanced RI and prolonged the post-intervention effect to 30 min, which is the longest post-intervention effect achieved with the shortest intervention time (10 min) among the RI-enhancing methods that have been investigated. These findings will facilitate the development of an effective and efficient intervention method for clinical application.
The joint movement function evaluation in Experiment 2 revealed that the joint movement function decreased under the forward attention condition and the monitor attention condition and was improved under the ankle joint attention condition. In addition, this is the first study to evaluate joint movement function as a post-intervention effect of the RPM RI enhancement method. In the forward attention and monitor attention conditions, the RI was increased by the RPM intervention, but the joint movement function decreased, contrary to our hypothesis. However, under the ankle joint attention condition, the RI was increased by the RPM intervention, and the joint movement function was improved, supporting our hypothesis. The reason for the decrease in joint movement function under the forward attention and monitor attention conditions could be the decrease in the corticospinal tract excitability of the agonist muscle, which may be caused by post-exercise depression (PED). It has been reported that PED is induced by non-fatigue active movement and passive movement [45,46,47,48,49,50,51]. PED is caused by the frequent repetition of activity in the primary motor area (M1) and induced by increased SICI, a measure of GABAergic intracortical inhibitory circuit excitability [50,51,52]. It has been reported that SICI is modulated by input from the proprioceptors [53]. The results of this study suggest that RPM-induced proprioceptor activity increased SICI, inhibited M1 excitability, and decreased the joint movement function.
Under the ankle joint attention condition, TA EMG increased during dorsiflexion after RPM intervention, the CI decreased due to the decrease in Sol EMG, and the joint movement function improved due to the increase in ankle dorsiflexion PT and RJD. The results of this study suggest that in addition to the RI enhancement (antagonist muscle EMG decrease during joint movement) by RPM, attention to the RPM intervention target may have increased M1 activity and thus the muscle output of the main movement muscle, improving the joint movement function. In a previous study [32], focusing attention on the index finger during RPM of the index finger increased the MEP and the excitability of the corticospinal tract. Corticospinal tract excitability is caused by a decrease in SAI due to attention [44] because SAI modulates the effect of somatosensory input on motor cortex excitability [54]. Studies have shown that SICI decreases with attention [43], and SICI is involved in the GABAergic inhibition mechanism [55, 56]. Therefore, attention to the ankle joint during RPM may have increased the excitability of M1 by decreasing the excitability of intracortical inhibitory circuits, which activated the main action muscle (TA) during the ankle dorsiflexion task and inhibited the antagonist muscle (Sol) by enhancing RI. Thus, the joint movement function was improved by decreasing CI and increasing ankle dorsiflexion PT.
One limitation of this study was that the excitability of the corticospinal tract corresponding to the muscle targeted for RPM intervention could not be evaluated. However, in previous studies [32, 52, 57], the excitability of corticospinal tracts and spinal anterior horn cells after RPM was evaluated and examined under many parameters, including different speeds and numbers of RPM movements and attention, and we believe that the findings support the results of this study. The results of the joint movement function evaluation in this study suggest that similar corticospinal tract excitability results were obtained during ankle joint RPM as in the previous study.
Clinical application
RPM is an effective and efficient RI enhancement intervention method. In addition, the findings of this study suggest that simply focusing attention on the RPM intervention target can enhance RI potentiation and improve joint movement function, which may be an effective adjunct therapy for functional recovery after central nervous system injury. RI-enhancing methods other than RPM have only shown effects for 10Â min after interventions of 15Â min or longer [14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. In this study, the participant was asked to focus their attention on the intervention target during RPM, and a 10Â min intervention resulted in a 30-min RI enhancement. Prolonging the duration of the intervention effect using adjunct therapy may improve rehabilitation. Future studies should investigate the post-intervention effect and joint movement function, gait, and balance function in patients.
Conclusions
In this study, we examined RI and joint movement function after RPM intervention, during which the participant’s attention was focused on different targets. As a result, it was clarified that the RPM intervention effect could be improved by focusing the participant’s attention on the intervention target (in this case, the ankle joint) during the 10-min RPM. Furthermore, the RI enhancement lasted for 30 min after the intervention, and the joint movement function improved.
Availability of data and materials
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, [R.H.], upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- CI:
-
Contraction index
- MVC:
-
Maximum voluntary contraction
- PED:
-
Post-exercise depression
- PES:
-
Patterned electrical stimulation
- PT:
-
Peak torque
- RI:
-
Reciprocal inhibition
- RJD:
-
Rate of joint movement development
- RPM:
-
Repetitive passive movement
- SAI:
-
Short-latency afferent inhibition
- SD:
-
Standard deviations
- SICI:
-
Short-interval intracortical inhibition
- TA:
-
Tibialis anterior
References
Hayashi A, Kagamihara Y, Nakajima Y, Narabayashi H, Okuma Y, Tanaka R. Disorder in reciprocal innervation upon initiation of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 1988;70:437–40.
Kagamihara Y, Ohi K, Tanaka R. Disorder in reciprocal inhibition upon initiation of voluntary ankle dorsiflexion movement in cerebellar motor disorders. In M. Mano, I. Hamada, & M. Delong (Eds.), Role of the cerebellum and basal ganglia in voluntary movement. 1993;33–40. New York, NY: Elsevier Science.
Kagamihara Y, Tanaka R. Disorder of the reciprocal Ia inhibitory pathway in spasticity. In: Recent Advances in Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;859–62.
Nagai K, Yamada M, Uemura K, Yamada Y, Ichihashi N, Tsuboyama T. Differences in muscle coactivation during postural control between healthy older and young adults. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2011;53:338–43.
Hortobagyi T, Solnik S, Gruber A, Rider P, Steinweg K, Helseth J, et al. Interaction between age and gait velocity in the amplitude and timing of antagonist muscle coactivation. Gait Posture. 2009;29:558–64.
Hortobagyi T, Devita P. Mechanisms responsible for the age-associated increase in coactivation of antagonist muscles. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2006;34:29–35.
Morita H, Shindo M, Yanagawa S, Yoshida T, Momoi H, Yanagisawa N. Progressive decrease in heteronymous monosynaptic Ia facilitation with human ageing. Exp Brain Res. 1995;104:167–70.
Morita H, Shindo M, Ikeda S, Yanagisawa N. Decrease in presynaptic inhibition on heteronymous monosynaptic Ia terminals in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2000;15:830–4.
Baudry S, Maerz AH, Enoka RM. Presynaptic modulation of Ia afferents in young and old adults when performing force and position control. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103:623–31.
Bhagchandani N, Schindler-Ivens S. Reciprocal inhibition post-stroke is related to reflex excitability and movement ability. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123:2239–46.
Fung J, Barbeau H. A dynamic EMG profile index to quantify muscular activation disorder in spastic paretic gait. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1989;73:233–44.
Hirabayashi R, Edama M, Kojima S, Miyaguchi S, Onishi H. Effects of repetitive passive movement on ankle joint on spinal reciprocal inhibition. Exp Brain Res. 2019;237:3409–17.
Hirabayashi R, Edama M, Kojima S, Miyaguchi S, Onishi H. Enhancement of spinal reciprocal inhibition depends on the movement speed and range of repetitive passive movement. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;52:3929–43.
Hirabayashi R, Kojima S, Edama M, Onishi H. Activation of the supplementary motor areas enhances spinal reciprocal inhibition in healthy individuals. Brain Sci. 2020;10(9):587.
Perez MA, Field-Fote EC, Floeter MK. Patterned sensory stimulation induces plasticity in reciprocal ia inhibition in humans. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2014–8.
Roche N, Lackmy A, Achache V, Bussel B, Katz R. Impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on spinal network excitability in humans. J Physiol. 2009;587:5653–64.
Fujiwara T, Tsuji T, Honaga K, Hase K, Ushiba J, Liu M. Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates the spinal plasticity induced with patterned electrical stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:1834–7.
Roche N, Lackmy A, Achache V, Bussel B, Katz R. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the leg motor area on lumbar spinal network excitability in healthy subjects. J Physiol. 2011;589:2813–26.
Jessop T, DePaola A, Casaletto L, Englard C, Knikou M. Short-term plasticity of human spinal inhibitory circuits after isometric and isotonic ankle training. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013;113:273–84.
Kubota S, Hirano M, Morishita T, Uehara K, Funase K. Patterned sensory nerve stimulation enhances the reactivity of spinal Ia inhibitory interneurons. NeuroReport. 2015;26:249–53.
Yamaguchi T, Fujiwara T, Tsai YA, Tang SC, Kawakami M, Mizuno K, et al. The effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation and patterned electrical stimulation on spinal inhibitory interneurons and motor function in patients with spinal cord injury. Exp Brain Res. 2016;234:1469–78.
Ritzmann R, Krause A, Freyler K, Gollhofer A. Acute whole-body vibration increases reciprocal inhibition. Hum Mov Sci. 2018;60:191–201.
Yamaguchi T, Fujiwara T, Lin SC, Takahashi Y, Hatori K, Liu M, et al. Priming with intermittent theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation promotes spinal plasticity induced by peripheral patterned electrical stimulation. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:508.
Takahashi Y, Fujiwara T, Yamaguchi T, Kawakami M, Mizuno K, Liu M. The effects of patterned electrical stimulation combined with voluntary contraction on spinal reciprocal inhibition in healthy individuals. NeuroReport. 2017;28:434–8.
Takahashi Y, Kawakami M, Yamaguchi T, Idogawa Y, Tanabe S, Kondo K, et al. Effects of leg motor imagery combined with electrical stimulation on plasticity of corticospinal excitability and spinal reciprocal inhibition. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:149.
Passingham RE. Attention to action. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;351:1473–9.
Hazeltine E, Grafton ST, Ivry R. Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. Brain. 1997;120:123–40.
Liao CM, Masters RS. Analogy learning: a means to implicit motor learning. J Sports Sci. 2001;19:307–19.
Wulf G, Lewthwaite R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23:1382–414.
Jueptner M, Stephan KM, Frith CD, Brooks DJ, Frackowiak RS, Passingham RE. Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1313–24.
Rowe J, Friston K, Frackowiak R, Passingham R. Attention to action: specific modulation of corticocortical interactions in humans. Neuroimage. 2002;17:988–98.
Tsuiki S, Sasaki R, Pham MV, Miyaguchi S, Kojima S, Saito K, et al. Repetitive passive movement modulates corticospinal excitability: effect of movement and rest cycles and subject attention. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13:38.
Hermens HJ, Freriks B, Disselhorst-Klug C, Rau G. Development of recommendations for SEMG sensors and sensor placement procedures. J Electromyogr Kinesiol. 2000;10:361–74.
Crone C, Hultborn H, Jespersen B, Nielsen J. Reciprocal Ia inhibition between ankle flexors and extensors in man. J Physiol. 1987;389:163–85.
Takada Y, Miyahara T, Tanaka T, Ohyama T, Nakamura Y. Modulation of H reflex of pretibial muscles and reciprocal Ia inhibition of soleus muscle during voluntary teeth clenching in humans. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2063–70.
Mizuno Y, Tanaka R, Yanagisawa N. Reciprocal group I inhibition on triceps surae motoneurons in man. J Neurophysiol. 1971;34:1010–7.
Nielsen J, Kagamihara Y. The regulation of disynaptic reciprocal Ia inhibition during co-contraction of antagonistic muscles in man. J Physiol. 1992;456:373–91.
Falconer K, Winter DA. Quantitative assessment of co-contraction at the ankle joint in walking. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;25:135–49.
Jankowska E, McCrea D, Rudomin P, Sykova E. Observations on neuronal pathways subserving primary afferent depolarization. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:506–16.
Meunier S, Pierrot-Deseilligny E. Cortical control of presynaptic inhibition of Ia afferents in humans. Exp Brain Res. 1998;119:415–26.
Nielsen J, Petersen N, Deuschl G, Ballegaard M. Task-related changes in the effect of magnetic brain stimulation on spinal neurones in man. J Physiol. 1993;471:223–43.
Masakado Y, Muraoka Y, Tomita Y, Chino N. The effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on reciprocal inhibition in the human leg. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;41:429–32.
Yamaguchi T, Moriya K, Tanabe S, Kondo K, Otaka Y, Tanaka S. Transcranial direct-current stimulation combined with attention increases cortical excitability and improves motor learning in healthy volunteers. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2020;17:23.
Mirdamadi JL, Suzuki LY, Meehan SK. Attention modulates specific motor cortical circuits recruited by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroscience. 2017;359:151–8.
Teo WP, Rodrigues JP, Mastaglia FL, Thickbroom GW. Changes in corticomotor excitability and inhibition after exercise are influenced by hand dominance and motor demand. Neuroscience. 2012;210:110–7.
Teo WP, Rodrigues JP, Mastaglia FL, Thickbroom GW. Post-exercise depression in corticomotor excitability after dynamic movement: a general property of fatiguing and non-fatiguing exercise. Exp Brain Res. 2012;216:41–9.
Zanette G, Bonato C, Polo A, Tinazzi M, Manganotti P, Fiaschi A. Long-lasting depression of motor-evoked potentials to transcranial magnetic stimulation following exercise. Exp Brain Res. 1995;107:80–6.
Bonato C, Zanette G, Polo A, Bongiovanni G, Manganotti P, Tinazzi M, et al. Cortical output modulation after rapid repetitive movements. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1994;15:489–94.
Teo WP, Rodrigues JP, Mastaglia FL, Thickbroom GW. Modulation of corticomotor excitability after maximal or sustainable-rate repetitive finger movement is impaired in Parkinson’s disease and is reversed by levodopa. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014;125:562–8.
Miyaguchi S, Onishi H, Kojima S, Sugawara K, Tsubaki A, Kirimoto H, et al. Corticomotor excitability induced by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation with and without non-exhaustive movement. Brain Res. 2013;1529:83–91.
Miyaguchi S, Kojima S, Kirimoto H, Tamaki H, Onishi H. Do differences in levels, types, and duration of muscle contraction have an effect on the degree of post-exercise depression? Front Hum Neurosci. 2016 Apr 29;10:159.
Sasaki R, Nakagawa M, Tsuiki S, Miyaguchi S, Kojima S, Saito K, et al. Regulation of primary motor cortex excitability by repetitive passive finger movement frequency. Neuroscience. 2017;357:232–40.
Rosenkranz K, Rothwell JC. Differential effect of muscle vibration on intracortical inhibitory circuits in humans. J Physiol. 2003;551:649–60.
Tokimura H, Di Lazzaro V, Tokimura Y, Oliviero A, Profice P, Insola A, et al. Short latency inhibition of human hand motor cortex by somatosensory input from the hand. J Physiol. 2000;523:503–13.
Ziemann U, Lönnecker S, Steinhoff BJ, Paulus W. Effects of antiepileptic drugs on motor cortex excitability in humans: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Ann Neurol. 1996;40:367–78.
Ziemann U, Lönnecker S, Steinhoff BJ, Paulus W. The effect of lorazepam on the motor cortical excitability in man. Exp Brain Res. 1996;109:127–35.
Sasaki R, Tsuiki S, Miyaguchi S, Kojima S, Saito K, Inukai Y, et al. Repetitive Passive Finger Movement Modulates Primary Somatosensory Cortex Excitability. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:332.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Enago (www.enago.jp) for the English language review.
Funding
This work was supported by the Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (20K19464) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) and Grant-in-Aid for Research A (R03C45) from the Niigata University of Health and Welfare, 2021.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
RH, ME, and HO designed the study. RH wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. RH, ME, MT, YY, HY, CS, and HO contributed to the analysis and interpretation of data and assisted in the preparation of the manuscript. All other authors have contributed to data collection and interpretation and critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work, ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants provided written informed consent to participate in this study. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the Niigata University of Health and Welfare (18267–190918). All experiments were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Niigata University of Health and Welfare and the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Hirabayashi, R., Edama, M., Takeda, M. et al. Participant attention on the intervention target during repetitive passive movement improved spinal reciprocal inhibition enhancement and joint movement function. Eur J Med Res 28, 428 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40001-023-01418-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40001-023-01418-7